So the percentage of lefthanders is
rising. This abstraction is from the 1951 printing. The first edition
was in 1937, the second in 1946, and the third in 1950.
Thus it is very likely that most of this dates from 1937.
When I’ve done checks, left-handedness is nearer
to 10%, and I did not notice a sex difference, but I was not very interested!
I expect that, like most statistics, the values vary in different populations.
Burt comes to the belief that people are not really
dvided by left- and right-handedness, but the left and right hand work
together as a team, with the traditionally nominated, dominate hand being
assigned to tasks requiring precision, and the other, more specialised,
being used for strength or steadying purposes. One hand writes while the
other steadies the paper, one hand guides the broom, while the other anchors
it.
Burt notes that more tics, nervousness and other problems
in left-handers. On page 290, he refers to, “[...] the older practice
which maintained that any child holding a pen in his left hand should
have his knuckles rapped [...] ”, but does not make the rather obvious
connection between this sort of action and other emotional difficulties.
CHAPTER
X
LEFT -HANDEDNESS
I. Methods, (definitions. The whole
section on left-handedness comprises 90 pages: pp. 270 - 359.)
Complexity of the Problem.—Of all the special motor disabilities
found among school children, that which interferes most widely with the
ordinary tasks of the classroom is left-handedness; and no question is
put by the teacher to the school psychologist more frequently than this:
‘How should I deal with a left-handed pupil?’ A condition
so common and so perplexing will require examination at some length.
One fact must be emphasized at the outset. Left-handedness is by no
means so definite a characteristic as is popularly supposed. Right-handedness
and left-handedness are relative terms - indeed, somewhat ambiguous terms,
not sharply defined alternatives, mutually exclusive and absolutely opposed.
To describe a child as left-handed without further explanatory details
conveys very little information about his manual habits generally and
still less about his neuro-muscular co-ordination as a whole. Usually
it simply means that the teacher has observed the child regularly writing
with his left hand, and is tempted to infer some inherent and abnormal
asymmetry in his nervous organization or brain.
Closer inquiry soon reveals that left-handedness can manifest itself
in very different forms and with very different degrees of strength. Moreover,
the mixed forms and the milder degrees prove quite as common as the thoroughgoing
or extreme. How many persons are consistently left-handed—or, for
that matter, consistently right-handed—for every transaction in
which hands are required? We shall find ample evidence to show that, quite
apart from any general inclination such as might be ascribed to an innate
or hereditary tendency, individual preferences in this movement or in
that are dictated now by habit, now by special circumstance, and now by
the intrinsic delicacy of the muscular co-ordinations involved. Accordingly,
mere casual observation of some particular stereotyped action, like writing
or drawing, will be of little value by itself: such observations need
to be supplemented by an inquiry into the conditions under which the action
has been learnt, and by specially devised tests which will estimate the
strength of any original or ineradicable bias.
Definition and Tests.—Before we can devise a proper test,
we must clarify our conception of what left-handedness denotes. An exact
definition is essential. By left-handedness [1] I understand
a consistent tendency (whether congenital, or induced post-natally by
accident or by some other change in the hand or its neuro-muscular apparatus)
to undertake new dexterities with the left hand rather than with the right.
It must be judged, therefore, not so much by long-standing habits as by
an unfamiliar task, and not by a single action, but by several. The points
to observe are not merely the child's customary mode of using the pencil
or pen, but his power to throw a ball or pick up a weight, to hammer or
bore, to sort marbles, deal cards, cut with a knife or scissors, stir
with a teaspoon in a cup, turn a handle or wind cotton round a reel, more
easily with the one hand than with the other. Probably the best single
test for rapid use is to ask the child to cut paper with loose-riveted
scissors. [2] Incidentally it is at times instructive
to make a note of any half-unconscious manual habits that are not influenced
by social pressure. For example, in clasping the hands, which thumb is
placed on top? And perhaps a little more significant-in folding the arms,
which hand is placed on top of the opposite arm?
If quantitative measurements are required, any of the tests of manual
dexterity described above may be employed. The child carries out the test
first with the hand that he usually writes with, and then with the other.
The index of left-handedness most commonly adopted (generally, but inaccurately,
termed ‘index of right-handedness’ or ‘dextrality’)
is given by the simple formula L/R x 100, where R and L denote the number
of marks scored with the right hand and with the left respectively; a
somewhat better measure of right-handedness is given by the formula R-L/R+L.
So important is it to detect tendencies towards left-handedness at the
earliest possible moment that a word or two may be added on the testing
of young infants. The critical period lies between six and fifteen months.
At this tender age a rough ‘reacting test’ is the easiest
to apply. Provisionally, in default of first-hand studies in this country,
I suggest that the experimenter should adopt the procedure and the norms
worked out by Gesell in America. ‘A red rod’ (e.g.
a red pencil) ‘is held in the median plane, and the child is encouraged
to make repeated efforts to grasp it’. With the shy, the dull, and
the mentally deficient, a sweet wrapped in coloured paper is sometimes
more effective. The examiner is also instructed to note ‘whether
the child uses one hand independently in his own spontaneous manipulations’—e.g.
in picking things up or flinging them down.[3]
Right- and Left-handedness in Two-handed Operations.—With
older children more recent investigators have urged that tests such as
I have mentioned, which they regard as tests of uni-manual activities
only, should be accompanied by tests and inquiries upon bi-manual activities.
These they divide into two sub-groups on a basis which at first sight
seems purely empirical. The typical activities chosen are (i) throwing
a ball (for the uni-manual test), and (for the bi-manual tests) (ii) using
a cricket bat, a golf club, or an axe, and (iii) using a pitchfork, a
shovel, or a broom—the use of the bat and the broom being generally
taken as the most representative. A ‘handedness formula’ is
then proposed which will indicate, by three letters, each individual’s
habits in each of the three directions. Thus if he (i) throws with his
right hand, (ii) bats with his left, and (iii) sweeps and digs with his
right, he is classed as R L R. But what are we to regard as ‘right-handed’
batting or sweeping, when by hypothesis both hands are used? The criterion
proposed is the hand which is held ‘nearer the business end of the
instrument’—that is, the lower end, the end farthest from
the shoulders.
To me this latter principle appears quite mistaken. The real question
is-which of the two hands executes the greatest movement and consequently
undertakes the more delicate and more active task of guiding or directing
the instrument? In batting, as in wielding a hockey stick or golf club,
the fulcrum is near the body, and the ‘business end’ executes
the wider movement. The right-handed bats man, therefore, almost always
places his right hand below the left. But in sweeping or digging it is
usually the nearer end of the broom or spade that is chiefly moved. With
the broom, it is true, various methods may be adopted. But in digging
the movements are fairly uniform: the left hand generally holds the middle
of the spade and supplies a fulcrum; the right hand moves the handle to
balance the heavy earth at the other end. Should we call the ordinary
method of using a billiard cue ‘left-handed’ because the left
hand is ‘nearer the business end’? With each of these instruments,
apart from special conditions (e.g. sweeping in awkward corners),
the dominating tendency is for the operator to work always on the same
side of the body. Thus a right-handed person keeps the handle of the bat,
broom, or spade mainly on his right. It follows that the relative positions
of the two hands on the handle will naturally be inverted for swinging
and for thrusting movements respectively—for swinging as in using
a cricket bat, and for thrusting as in sweeping, digging, or striking
with a spear or pushing with a cue.
If these observations are correct, the third letter in the usual ‘handedness
formula’ should evidently be reversed: R R L should be called R
R R, and so throughout the series. And, in point of fact, the figures
given by all investigators show that, in using the spade or broom, the
vast majority of persons, who are otherwise right-handed, place the left hand down—i.e. ‘nearer the business end’. It
seems wholly illogical to designate this a left-handed procedure.
Manual Types.—Those who lay stress on the difference
between bi-manual and uni-manual operations believe that we must recognize
a number of clear-cut types. Instead of splitting the whole population
into two simple categories, the right-handed and the left-handed respectively,
there are, they assert, at least half a dozen subdivisions. Each type
is different in nature, and calls for a different treatment in the classroom.
Hence they argue that all the earlier work on left-handedness as it affects
the child at school is invalidated from the outset. It over-simplifies
the problem.
Their detailed classification follows from the formula given above.
A twofold division, as we have seen, is applied and re-applied on three
successive principles. In theory this should furnish 23 = 8
possible types. But in practice, it is said, ‘there is no such combination
as R L R and L R L .... When a man is seen throwing with the right hand
and batting with the left hand, then it is certain that he will sweep
and pitch hay with the left hand.’ Hence we are left with only six.[5]
The suggestion emanates from America, where most of the more recent
inquiries have been carried out. To check their deductions, and to verify
my own hypotheses, I have collected the following data from English adults.
The right-handed persons were mainly post-graduate students at a training
college; and to obtain a larger number of left-handed types I have extended
my investigations to all the alleged cases of left-handedness I have encountered
at public lectures or in private life. The following table summarizes
the chief results (Table XVIII)
TABLE XVIII. FREQUENCY
OF VARIOUS RIGHT-HANDED AND LEFT-HANDED TYPES |
Right-handed for Uni-manual Operations. |
Left-handed for Uni-manual Operations. |
Operation. |
Men. |
Women. |
Both sexes. |
Operation. |
Men. |
Women. |
Both sexes. |
i |
ii |
iii |
iv |
|
|
|
i |
ii |
iii |
iv |
|
|
|
R |
R |
R |
(L) |
47.0 |
51.2 |
49.9 |
L |
L |
L |
(R) |
26.5 |
40.3 |
32.2 |
R |
R |
L |
(R) |
42.3 |
29.1 |
35.9 |
L |
L |
R |
(L) |
24.1 |
31.6 |
27.1 |
R |
L |
R |
(L) |
8.8 |
14.8 |
11.7 |
L |
R |
L |
(R) |
44.6 |
24.6 |
36.4 |
R |
L |
L |
(R) |
1.9 |
4.9 |
3.4 |
L |
R |
R |
(L) |
4.8 |
3.5 |
4.3 |
Total: |
100.0 |
100.0 |
100.0 |
|
100.0 |
100.0 |
100.0 |
Number: |
215 |
203 |
418 |
|
83 |
57 |
140 |
Key |
i |
Throwing |
|
ii |
Batting |
|
iii |
Sweeping (Burt) |
|
iv |
Sweeping (Rife) |
At first sight the variety of combinations appears somewhat bewildering.
They are largely accounted for when we look more closely into the nature
of those two-handed activities that are taken as the basis of the sub-classification.
As we have already seen, the movements in the first set (using a bat or
a golf club) involve a swinging stroke, carefully aimed and skilfully
applied: they are, in fact, difficult and dexterous operations; moreover,
they are learnt and practised in teams or social groups; hence nonconforming
methods are apt to be quizzed and criticized until they are brought into
line. The activities in the second set (sweeping and digging) are by comparison
coarse and unskilled processes: as usually carried out, they involve a
push or thrust, and demand strength rather than precision. They are generally
undertaken alone; hence individual oddities may pass uncorrected.[6]
The following conclusions, suggested by the figures in the table, will
now become intelligible.
- The commonest type of all is the type that is consistent throughout.[7] The right-handed tend to be right-handed for all the operations tested;
the left-handed (except for male cricketers) tend to be left-handed.
But the tendency is far from universal: taken together, the exceptions
are more numerous than the rule.
- In the main, it is the degree of skill required, much more than the
number of hands involved, that determines how the hands shall be used.
Thus hand-preference is much more likely to vary in the different two-handed
operations, which may be either skilled or unskilled, than in different
skilled operations, which may be either one-handed or two-handed. It
follows that, among right-handed and left-handed alike, the preference
in skilled two-handed activities, such as batting, nearly always follows
the preference in one-handed activities, such as throwing. The only
noteworthy instance to the contrary is that of right-handed batting
among left-handed men, which is explicable by social pressure.
- Among the many who consequently employ the same hand for aIl difficult
or delicate movements, whether the total operation is of the one-handed
type or of the skilled two-handed type, the procedure adopted for operations
of the unskilled twohanded type is fairly well divided. Sorne keep the
skilled hand near the business end, so preserving the same position
for all two-handed operations, skilled or unskilled, and holding brooms
as though they were bats. Others, more appropriately, keep the skilled
hand near the guiding end—that is, near the free end of the handle,
and so follow out their one-handed tendency; this practice is commoner
among the women-no doubt because for them sweeping is a relatively skilled
operation.
- Among the right-handed, those who throw with one hand and bat with
the opposite are distinctly scarce, especially when the batsman is a
male. Among the left-handed they are more abundant, and, indeed, with
the males provide the commonest type. These latter are mainly men in
whom a left-handed tendency is present to a mild degree: for batting
they have learnt the orthodox right-handed fashion, but have kept or
cultivated a left-handed method for bowling, throwing, and tossmg.[8]
- Finally, when a person uses opposite hands for onehanded and for skilled
two-handed operations respectively, the procedure which is most consistent
with both of these practices is almost invariably adopted for unskilled
two-handed operations: e.g. the man who throws with the left
hand and bats with the right, will use his left hand to guide a broom
in sweeping and keep his right hand down on the handle-thus holding
the broom much as he holds a bat.
I conclude that we cannot regard these differences as signalizing fundamentally
distinct or basic types. The right- or left-handed aptitude, whichever
it may be, tends to dominate throughout, though in differing degrees
with different persons. When comparatively weak, it may be easily overcome
by circumstance or tradition, and may never. come definitely into play
in activities that require little skill and can be performed in various
ways according to the conditions of the moment (as, for example, wielding
an axe or a broom). When strong, it may influence every action.
How far do these considerations affect inquiries on children of school
age? Plainly, among boys and girls of the class we are studying, it
would be all. but useless to· attempt such classifications. Among
the elementary school population in London few of the girls play cricket
or hockey; and not many of the boys use a shovel or a broom. So far
as my observations go, the younger boys at cricket and the younger girls
in sweeping seem to follow almost without exception their one-handed
preferences. The older left-handed boys who play under the supervision
of a master are usually taught to comply with the traditional practice;
and the older girls who attend housewifery classes accept the methods
of mopping and sweeping in which they are instructed. Though the degree
of skill achieved by a left-handed child with a right-handed method
is admittedly a little poorer, in neither case does there seem to be
any irksome struggle in ma king the adaptations required. Doubtless
because no fine accuracy is imperatively demanded, there is nothing
like the conflicts that are observable in learning to use the pen. Accordingly,
if my view is right, these further subdivisions have no wide bearing
on the problem of the teacher, and, so far as children are concerned,
are of interest only here and there in analysing the general tendencies
of certain individuals. In every case what we really need are tests
of ease of learning, not of habits already learnt. I have, therefore,
not considered it necessary to supplement what some would regard as
uni-manual tests with extensive testing for bi-manual activities.[4] In the main I have contented myself with the methods and observations
described above.
Questionnaires and Group-tests.—For group inquiries a
questionnaire on the following lines will pick out about 90 per cent.
of those with left-handed tendencies.
‘Which hand would you use
- to write,
- to draw or paint,
- to throw a ball,
- to strike with a racket, stick, or bat,
- to hold a penknife,
- to cut with a pair of scissors,
- to carry a cup of water or lift a glass in drinking,
- to clean your teeth,
- to wind a clock or watch or musical-box,
- to reach a book or plate on a high shelf almost out of your reach?
Imagine yourself doing these things before you answer; and underline
the true reply’. The words ‘left’, ‘right’,
‘either hand’, are printed as alternative answers against
each question.
For group-tests in class the simpler of the tests of manual speed and
dexterity can be readily applied. In my own investigations the ‘tapping’,
‘aiming’, and ‘tracing’ tests were chiefly employed.[8]
end note
- The etymology of the word is suggestive. In the
present sense the word, ‘left’ is not connected, as is so
often supposed (e.g. by Webster’s Dictionary,
s.v.), with the verb ‘to leave’. It is derived from the
Anglo-Saxon ‘lyft’ = weak, broken (akin to ‘lopt’
or ‘lopped’ and possibly to the German ‘lieht’
and ‘leieht’, and to ‘light’ in the sense of
fragile). Thus to the Saxon the left arm meant the weak arm: to the
Latin and the Greek (if we may trust the usual derivation) the left
hand meant the shield hand (laeva, laia—archaic
and poetic words), or, later on, the pocket hand (sinister,
a less poetic word, from the ‘sinus’ of the toga). These
designations, as we shall see in a moment, might almost be taken as
emblematic of the two contrasting theories of the function of the left
hand in man.
- In their earlier experiments investigators relied
solely or mainly upon the dynamometer. The data collected by the Galton
laboratory and analysed by Pearson and Woo were based on strength of
grip; and similarly with much of the evidence in regard to primitive
races. But the dynamometer is primarily a test of strength; whereas
right- or left-handedness as I have defined it turns primarily on capacity
for skill. I find that as many as 41 per cent. of those who habitually
use the left hand for skilled actions nevertheless have a stronger grip
with the right (see also Whipple, loc. cit.).
- Mental Growth of the Pre-School Child,
p. 80. Something a little more precise and detailed, however, is urgently
needed in regard to both methods and results. Miss Woolley’s method
(loc. cit. inf.)—placing two coloured discs side by side
in front of the child—though adopted by many early investigators,
seems still less satisfactory. It yields one fact of theoretical interest:
with this procedure one can often observe a definite tendency to look
towards the disc on the right quite apart from any movement of the right
hand.
- The suggestion seems first to have been put forward
by J. Merle Rife (‘Types of Dextrality,’ Psycho. Rev.,
XXIX, 1922, pp. 474-81). An extended investigation along these lines,
made with the aid of a grant from the American National Research Council,
is described by June E. Downey, ‘Types of Dextrality and their
Implications,’ Am. Journ. Psych., XXXVIII, 1927 (pp.
317 et seq.).
- Rife, loc. cit., p. 4-77. No figures,
however, are given in support of this assertion. According to my own
results (cf, Table XVIII, last line),
the rejected combinations, though rare, are by no means non-existent.
- Generally speaking, with the unsophisticated sweeper
the natural movement in pushing a broom is rather like the movement
in pushing a billiard cue, a bayonet, or a spear. The butt end tends
to swing back past the body; the active hand is therefore nearer the
butt. That this is the main factor in determining the relative position
of the hands in sweeping may be seen on contrasting it with the opposite
motion of paddling. In paddling a canoe it is the blade and not the
handle that comes back past the body; consequently, the active hand
must now be nearer the blade, while the other hand rests on the butt
end. This resembles the position in batting, except that, since the
strong stroke is now a backward pull instead of a forward drive, the
palm curves round the front of the handle instead of round the back.
For efficient sweeping indoors, however, some skill is required; and
the movements, and consequently the hand positions, may often be altered
according to the part of the room to be swept. Indeed, many women consider
themselves ambidextrous for sweeping. Further, at housewifery classes,
sweeping is generally performed under criticism and instruction. Hence
girls tend to adopt a more conventional and therefore a more nearly
unanimous position. In sweeping indoors the girl is usually instructed
to pull or draw rather than to thrust or push, and to press and keep
the bristles down rather than to fling them (and the dust) up and away.
The position of the hands relative to each other will usually remain
the same; but (and this is a point that is missed by psychological writers)
the same position of each hand relative to the handle is now reversed:
the left palm is now nearly always placed over the handle,
and the right usually under it, because the ‘business end’
has to be pressed down—a motion which is given best by the left
hand with right-handed persons. In lifting the ‘business end’
of such an instrument up (as in using a pitchfork) the left palm will
be under the handle and the right usually above it. In thrusting
(e.g. in striking with a spear) the palms are usually at the
side. Observe that, when a right-handed man thrusts, the positions of
his hands, both as regards each other and as regards the surface of
the handle, are very similar to those adopted when a left-handed man
bats. Hence the man’s method of holding the more familiar bat
often influences him when he comes to handle the less familiar broom,
for the male method of sweeping is generally to thrust or shove: and
thus he is sometimes tempted to sweep left-handed. Similarly, the woman’s
method of holding the broom often influences her when she comes to pick
up a bat. These somewhat thoughtless and awkward positions, assumed
with unfamiliar instruments, are especially noticeable among those who
are less efficient in novel activities of every kind, i.e. among the
less intelligent.
- This may seem at first sight to contradict Rife’s
statement that ‘the type which throws, bats, and uses the spade
right-handed ... seems to be the least common’ (pp. 474-5); but
it must be remembered that what Rife terms a right-handed use of the
spade is here regarded as left-handed.
- In golf, it may be noted, and in mowing, the
right-handed form of the club and scythe often compels the learner to
follow the traditional method. It is true that golf clubs and cricket
bats exist for the left-handed; but I have never heard of a left-handed
scythe.
- A questionnaire and a series of such tests, specially
devised for discovering left-handedness, together with the results obtained
by their use, have recently been described by Durost, loc.cit.sup.
- The backward child
by Cyril Burt,
University of London Press Ltd, 1951
[first published 1937]
quote taken from pp270 - 280 of 3rd edition. reprint.
|